idw – Informationsdienst Wissenschaft

Nachrichten, Termine, Experten

Grafik: idw-Logo
Science Video Project
idw-Abo

idw-News App:

AppStore

Google Play Store



Instance:
Share on: 
01/12/2024 11:24

Dataset bias in machine learned theories of economic decisions

Bettina Bastian Science Communication Centre - Abteilung Kommunikation
Technische Universität Darmstadt

    Researchers at the Centre for Cognitive Science at TU Darmstadt and hessian.AI investigate the properties of behavioral economic theories automatically learned by AI in a publication in "Nature Human Behaviour".

    In our daily lives, we are constantly confronted with risky choices: whether to buy a new phone that might go on sale tomorrow, play the lottery, invest in the stock market, or take out insurance. Economists, psychologists, and cognitive scientists have long studied people’s risky choices in the laboratory with equivalent gambles because gambles have the three components that define risky choices: there is a choice between alternative actions, the outcomes of choices have a certain probability, and the outcomes have payoffs. Would you rather choose 100€ with certainty or get a lottery ticket, where the chances of winning 150€ are 75%, but in the remaining 25% you get nothing? The results of such experiments have shown consistently that people systematically deviate from the mathematical optimum choice, i.e., they lose money. In the above-mentioned game of chance, for example, the second option has a higher expected value, but when asked, many people prefer the first option.
    Due to the serious impact of these decision errors on the lives of individuals and the economy as a whole, predicting how humans make suboptimal decisions is of ongoing scientific interest. The work of Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky, who stated they were studying " natural stupidity instead of artificial intelligence", led to a better description of human decision-making through Cumulative Prospect Theory and the awarding of the Nobel Prize in Economic Sciences in 2002. However, there are still many anomalies and contexts in which decisions cannot be predicted well. Beyond predicting people's decisions, it is even more difficult to find explanations for them. This includes prominent ideas such as Gerd Gigerenzer's that human decisions are based on cognitive shortcuts, so-called heuristics.

    In a recent study (Peterson et al., 2021, Science), a research team from Princeton University, USA, used artificial intelligence to better understand human decisions in risky gambles. The idea: if deep neural networks become increasingly successful at predicting data, they could perhaps also predict human decisions better than behavioral economic theories. However, such neural networks require huge amounts of data in order to be trained. For this reason, a data set containing human decisions on more than 13,000 bets was collected as part of the study. When predicting people's gambling decisions based on this data set, the neural networks that were least constrained by theoretical assumptions performed best. Peterson et al. then derived a "machine-learned theory of economic decision-making" - an interpretable summary of the neural networks' behavior.

    In a new publication in the international journal "Nature Human Behavior", researchers from the LOEWE project "WhiteBox" at TU Darmstadt have systematically investigated the predictions that result from combining different machine learning models with different decision data sets. The publication, which originated in a course project in the Master's program in Cognitive Science, found striking differences in the prediction of human decisions: While some neural networks were most accurate at predicting decisions from the 2021 study dataset, they failed to predict human behavior from smaller psychological experiments. This is a typical example of how biases in data sets can lead to interaction effects between models and data sets, so that findings from one data set do not transfer to another. From these observations, the researchers were able to develop a cognitive generative model that quantitatively explains the differences between the actual decisions from the data sets and the predictions of the AI models with classical behavioral economic results of human decision uncertainty.

    "Neural networks may outperform all proposals by human theorists in terms of prediction errors on a dataset, but that does not guarantee that this transfers to other datasets of human gambles, or even more naturalistic, everyday decisions", explains Professor Constantin Rothkopf from the Centre for Cognitive Science at TU. The study underlines that cognitive science still cannot easily be automated by artificial intelligence and that a careful combination of theoretical reasoning, machine learning and data analysis are required to understand and explain why human decisions deviate from the mathematical optimum.


    Contact for scientific information:

    Tobias Thomas M.Sc.
    Psychologie der Informationsverarbeitung
    tobias.thomas@tu-darmstdt.de

    +49 6151 16-23366

    S1|15 234
    Alexanderstraße 10
    64283 Darmstadt


    Original publication:

    Thomas, T., Straub, D., Tatai, F. et al. Modelling dataset bias in machine-learned theories of economic decision-making. Nat Hum Behav (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01784-6


    Images

    Criteria of this press release:
    Journalists, Scientists and scholars
    Information technology, Psychology
    transregional, national
    Research results
    English


     

    Help

    Search / advanced search of the idw archives
    Combination of search terms

    You can combine search terms with and, or and/or not, e.g. Philo not logy.

    Brackets

    You can use brackets to separate combinations from each other, e.g. (Philo not logy) or (Psycho and logy).

    Phrases

    Coherent groups of words will be located as complete phrases if you put them into quotation marks, e.g. “Federal Republic of Germany”.

    Selection criteria

    You can also use the advanced search without entering search terms. It will then follow the criteria you have selected (e.g. country or subject area).

    If you have not selected any criteria in a given category, the entire category will be searched (e.g. all subject areas or all countries).