idw - Informationsdienst
Wissenschaft
Our brains construct mental maps of the environment from the experiences of our senses. This allows us to orient ourselves, remember where something happened, and plan where we go next. Researchers at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig and the Kavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience in Trondheim have now developed a new computer model that can finely watch the brain as it orients in space and remembers things.
In their recent publication in Nature Communications, they show that newly formed memories affect how we perceive the world around us: the more familiar our environment is, the fewer information needs to be integrated. This is directly reflected in our brain activity and can now be measured.
The remarkable ability of our brains to form maps of the environment and retrieve them on demand requires several complex neural calculations. Many of these are executed along a neural pathway that extends from the visual cortex to higher-level memory centers in our brain. To understand how individual brain areas along this pathway work together to control our behavior, Matthias Nau and Christian Doeller from the MPI CBS have developed an algorithm, together with their colleagues from Norway. This so-called encoding model enables them to directly link the brain activity of participants with their behavior and their ongoing memory formation. In particular, the team wanted to know how the human brain maps the environment during a natural and very complex behavior - namely spatial navigation.
Natural behavior observed in a virtual environment
To obtain a broad perspective on the human brain, the scientists used functional magnetic resonance imaging - a technique that can measure the activity of the entire brain via blood flow. The study participants were placed in a high field MRI scanner (7-tesla ) and navigated in a virtual arena using a keyboard similar to playing a computer game. They were asked to memorize the positions of hidden objects. "In order to understand how our participants oriented themselves in this task, we analyzed how their brain activity reflected the direction they were moving, within the virtual world, at any given time," explains Matthias Nau. Without a sense of direction this task would not have been possible. "We built an encoding model of their facing direction to estimate the influence of direction on neuronal activity in each part of the brain. Using this model, we were then able to predict, how the activity would unfold in new data from the same participant." To do this, the algorithm simulated not just one, but many possible versions of how each brain area might represent direction. The researchers identified which version of the model best predicted new data and were thus able to map previously unknown details of the directional code throughout the brain.
Brain activity reflects behavior controlled by memory
The novel analysis method made it possible to measure human perception of direction with unprecedented detail. The scientists not only observed an increase in brain activity in regions that filter spatial information from visual stimuli, but were also able to find clues, particularly in the higher memory regions, as to how well the participants remembered the locations of the objects they were looking for.
The results suggest that the process of encoding the world and the objects in it, i.e. cognitive mapping, influences how the entire network of regions in the brain processes information that we are currently deriving from our environment. The interaction of different visual and memory-forming brain areas is thus much more directly related to memory-controlled behavior than has been known up to now. We typically think of memory as discrete events from our past that we can call to mind. But why do we have memories in the first place? They enable us to learn from our past and to adapt our behavior accordingly in the future. Here, the scientists show that our memories have a direct influence on how we perceive the world around us, and that this interplay between perception and memory guides our behavior in everyday life.
The scientists are making the newly developed method available to the scientific community as an open-access analysis tool so that it can also be applied to other data, such as electrical brain data (EEG), or even in animal models. In this way, the neuronal processes involved in memory formation and spatial navigation can be investigated even more comprehensively.
Matthias Nau
Doktorand
matthias.nau@ntnu.no
"Behavior-dependent directional tuning in the human visual-navigation network"
by Matthias Nau, Tobias Navarro Schröder, Markus Frey & Christian F. Doeller
in: Nature Communications (2020)
https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-020-17000-2
https://www.mpg.de/149614/kognition_neuro
Study participants navigated in a virtual arena using a keyboard similar to playing a computer game.
MPI CBS
Criteria of this press release:
Journalists, Scientists and scholars
Psychology
transregional, national
Research results
English
You can combine search terms with and, or and/or not, e.g. Philo not logy.
You can use brackets to separate combinations from each other, e.g. (Philo not logy) or (Psycho and logy).
Coherent groups of words will be located as complete phrases if you put them into quotation marks, e.g. “Federal Republic of Germany”.
You can also use the advanced search without entering search terms. It will then follow the criteria you have selected (e.g. country or subject area).
If you have not selected any criteria in a given category, the entire category will be searched (e.g. all subject areas or all countries).