idw - Informationsdienst
Wissenschaft
How is conceptual knowledge represented in the brain such that we can flexibly use it to interpret unfamiliar information or to infer relations we’ve never directly experienced? One means of organizing conceptual knowledge would be in a kind of internal map. Thus, the map would have to be dynamically defined along those feature dimensions that are currently relevant to the concept.
Stephanie Theves and Christian Doeller of MPI CBS in Leipzig together with Guillén Fernández of the Donders Institute Nijmegen, have now shown such a distinction between conceptually relevant and overall features for the mapping function of the hippocampus.
How is conceptual knowledge represented in the brain such that we can flexibly use it to interpret unfamiliar information or to infer relations we’ve never directly experienced? One means of organizing conceptual knowledge would be in a kind of internal map: For example, one might infer whether an unknown animal can fly based on its proximity to familiar animals in a space defined along the feature dimensions ‘body weight’ and ‘wing size’. However, a given animal exhibits many more features than just those relevant to its ability to fly. Thus, in order to use a map-like representation to transfer meaning (e.g. able to fly?) to novel information (e.g. an unknown animal) via similarity to familiar exemplars, the map would have to be dynamically defined along those feature dimensions that are currently relevant to the concept.
Stephanie Theves and Christian Doeller of MPI CBS in Leipzig together with Guillén Fernández of the Donders Institute Nijmegen, have now shown such a distinction between conceptually relevant and overall features for the mapping function of the hippocampus.
The group has led the way in showing that the hippocampal formation, originally considered the brain’s navigational system, encodes far more than just mental maps of our spatial environment. It can also organize more abstract, non-spatial information into map-like representations.
In the present study, participants learned to classify novel abstract objects (which varied in terms of the frequency of their stripes, dots, and opacity) into two categories, based on the ratio of only two of their three features. These two feature dimensions could be regarded like the x- and y-axis of a coordinate system, in which the diagonal reflects the category boundary. For instance, an object with relatively high opacity as compared to its dot frequency would belong into category B. Its stripe frequency was irrelevant here. Importantly, while only two of the objects’ three feature dimensions thus defined the concept of the two categories, all three object features had to be precisely memorized.
“The design of the study allowed us to investigate whether the hippocampus indeed forms a map-like representation of the concept in specific” explains lead author Stephanie Theves. Using brain activity patterns of the hippocampus, measured with functional MRI, along with sophisticated statistical analyses the team was able to confirm their hypothesis. Theves concludes “Our analyses indicate that the hippocampus integrates only those feature dimensions that are relevant in relation to each other, to define the concept in a map-like representation. In other words, even though specific exemplars were encoded and remembered on all dimensions, a conceptual representation seemed to be carved out from the totality of the features. Therefore, we assume that the hippocampus contributes to concept learning, by representing behaviorally-relevant information in low-dimensional spaces.”
Stephanie Theves
Postdoc
theves@cbs.mpg.de
The Hippocampus Maps Concept Space, Not Feature Space
Stephanie Theves, Guillén Fernández and Christian F. Doeller
Journal of Neuroscience 16 September 2020, 40 (38) 7318-7325;
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0494-20.2020
https://www.cbs.mpg.de/departments/psychology
The hippocampus contributes to concept learning, by representing behaviorally-relevant information i ...
MPI CBS
Criteria of this press release:
Journalists
Psychology
transregional, national
Research results
English
You can combine search terms with and, or and/or not, e.g. Philo not logy.
You can use brackets to separate combinations from each other, e.g. (Philo not logy) or (Psycho and logy).
Coherent groups of words will be located as complete phrases if you put them into quotation marks, e.g. “Federal Republic of Germany”.
You can also use the advanced search without entering search terms. It will then follow the criteria you have selected (e.g. country or subject area).
If you have not selected any criteria in a given category, the entire category will be searched (e.g. all subject areas or all countries).