idw – Informationsdienst Wissenschaft

Nachrichten, Termine, Experten

Grafik: idw-Logo
Grafik: idw-Logo

idw - Informationsdienst
Wissenschaft

Science Video Project
idw-Abo

idw-News App:

AppStore

Google Play Store



Instance:
Share on: 
07/18/2022 11:48

Tapping, Clapping, Dancing: The Genetic Architecture of Rhythm

Ina Wittmann Presse- und Öffentlichkeitsarbeit
Max-Planck-Institut für empirische Ästhetik

New Findings on the Evolutionary Origins of Beat Synchronization

When we hear a beat, we tend to move our bodies to it automatically. Is this tendency to move in time with musical rhythm genetically determined? An international research team involving Vanderbilt University in Nashville, USA, and the Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics (MPIEA) in Frankfurt am Main decided to investigate this question. The results of their extensive study, which included more than 600,000 research participants, have recently been published as an open-access article in the journal Nature Human Behaviour.

Representing ten research institutions in six countries, the interdisciplinary team consisted of experts in complex genetics, music cognition, evolutionary biology, evolution, music, and neuroscience*. Together with the biotech company 23andMe, Inc. (Sunnyvale, USA), they conducted the first large-scale genome-wide association study of a musical trait.

In all, 606,825 research participants reported on whether they could clap in time with a musical beat. To test the reliability of these self-reports, the researchers, led by Nori Jacoby of MPIEA, conducted a series of online experiments with a separate, smaller group of study participants using a new technology for measuring real-time tapping responses (REPP). While the participants listened to music via their own computers at home, the team used the computer microphones to record their tapping responses and determine exactly when they tapped in relation to the musical beat.

Jacoby elaborates: “The human ability to move in time with musical beat is called beat synchronization. Our validation experiments showed that participants’ self-assessments matched the beat synchronization that was objectively measured. The data obtained from the large sample was therefore reliable—although we were only assessing one question.”

The sizeable research dataset gave the scientists an opportunity to detect even small genetic signals. They were thus able to identify 69 independent genetic variants associated with beat synchronization. What this makes clear is that having a sense of rhythm is influenced by far more than just a single gene.

As it turns out, many of these genetic variants are located in or near genes involved in neuronal function and early brain development. The researchers also found that beat synchronization has some of the same genetic structures involved in biological rhythms such as walking or breathing. In addition, they discovered associations with a number of traits related to aging, such as lung function and motor ability.

“As we see with other complex traits, there are many genes of small effect—likely more than we could identify here—which together only partially account for differences in people’s capacities for rhythm; environment also plays a crucial role,” says Miriam A. Mosing of the MPIEA.

The study’s findings represent a major advance in our understanding of the links between genomics and musicality, but they preclude any deterministic conclusions.

To make sure the results are interpreted in a responsible way the authors align with other collaborators engaged in a companion paper discussing the societal and ethical implications, risks and possibilities enabled by this study (https://psyarxiv.com/dyn6e/).

This research was supported in part by an NIH Director’s New Innovator Award (#DP2HD098859).

* This paper was led by Maria Niarchou (1). Senior authors were Lea K. Davis (1, 2), Nori Jacoby (3) and Reyna L. Gordon (1, 2).

Full list of authors:
Maria Niarchou (1), Daniel E. Gustavson (1), J. Fah Sathirapongsasuti (4), Manuel Anglada-Tort (3), Else Eising (5), Eamonn Bell (6, 7), Evonne McArthur (1), Peter Straub (1), 23andMe Research Team (4), J. Devin McAuley (8), John A. Capra (9), Fredrik Ullén (5, 10), Nicole Creanza (2), Miriam A. Mosing (5, 10, 11), David A. Hinds (4), Lea K. Davis 1, 2), Nori Jacoby (5), and Reyna L. Gordon (1, 2)

1: Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA;
2: Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA;
3: Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany;
4: 23andMe, Inc, Sunnyvale, CA, USA;
5: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands;
6: Columbia University, New York, NY, USA;
7: Durham University, Durham, UK;
8: Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA;
9: University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA;
10: Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden;
11: University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia


Contact for scientific information:

Max-Planck-Institute for Empirical Aesthetics
Dr. Nori Jacoby
nori.jacoby@ae.mpg.de


Original publication:

Niarchou, M., Gustavson, D. E., Sathirapongsasuti, J. F., Anglada-Tort, M., Eising, E., Bell, E., McArthur, E., Straub, P., 23andMe Research Team, McAuley, J. D., Capra, J. A., Ullén, F. U., Creanza, N., Mosing, M. A., Hinds, D. A., Davis, L. K., Jacoby, N., & Gordon, R. L. (2022). Genome-Wide Association Study of Musical Beat Synchronization Demonstrates High Polygenicity. Nature Human Behaviour. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01359-x


Images

The sense of rhythm has some of the same genetic structures involved in biological rhythms such as walking or breathing.
The sense of rhythm has some of the same genetic structures involved in biological rhythms such as w ...

(Picture: MPI for Empirical Aesthetics/F. Bernoully)


Addendum from 07/18/2022

Footnote Correction:

* This paper was led by Maria Niarchou (1). Senior authors were Lea K. Davis (1, 2), Nori Jacoby (3) and Reyna L. Gordon (1, 2).

Full list of authors:
Maria Niarchou (1), Daniel E. Gustavson (1), J. Fah Sathirapongsasuti (4), Manuel Anglada-Tort (3), Else Eising (5), Eamonn Bell (6, 7), Evonne McArthur (1), Peter Straub (1), 23andMe Research Team (4), J. Devin McAuley (8), John A. Capra (9), Fredrik Ullén (3, 10), Nicole Creanza (2), Miriam A. Mosing (3, 10, 11), David A. Hinds (4), Lea K. Davis (1, 2), Nori Jacoby (3), and Reyna L. Gordon (1, 2)

1: Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA;
2: Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA;
3: Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany;
4: 23andMe, Inc, Sunnyvale, CA, USA;
5: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, Netherlands;
6: Columbia University, New York, NY, USA;
7: Durham University, Durham, UK;
8: Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA;
9: University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA;
10: Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden;
11: University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia


Criteria of this press release:
Journalists
Biology, Medicine, Music / theatre
transregional, national
Research results, Scientific Publications
English


 

Help

Search / advanced search of the idw archives
Combination of search terms

You can combine search terms with and, or and/or not, e.g. Philo not logy.

Brackets

You can use brackets to separate combinations from each other, e.g. (Philo not logy) or (Psycho and logy).

Phrases

Coherent groups of words will be located as complete phrases if you put them into quotation marks, e.g. “Federal Republic of Germany”.

Selection criteria

You can also use the advanced search without entering search terms. It will then follow the criteria you have selected (e.g. country or subject area).

If you have not selected any criteria in a given category, the entire category will be searched (e.g. all subject areas or all countries).