idw – Informationsdienst Wissenschaft

Nachrichten, Termine, Experten

Grafik: idw-Logo
Science Video Project
idw-Abo

idw-News App:

AppStore

Google Play Store



Instanz:
Teilen: 
02.09.2022 12:02

When humans gesture, they activate an ancient connection between movement and breathing

Dr. Fabienne Salfner Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft (ZAS)
Geisteswissenschaftliche Zentren Berlin e.V. (GWZ)

    Researchers from Radboud University and ZAS Berlin have developed a new perspective on why humans often move their hands rhythmically during speaking. The perspective published in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, suggests that there are striking connections between animal and human behavior when it comes to moving with sounds. For example, flying bats synchronize their echo-locating vocalization and their wingbeats which can be directly related to how humans synchronize acoustic features of their voice and move their upper limbs.

    “When a bat echo-locates while flying, the muscle tensioning that drive the wingbeats also leads to small compressions of the lungs. It becomes more economical for the bat to vocalize during these particular moments of the wingbeat cycle, leading to synchronization,” says researcher Wim Pouw at the Donders Center for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour. Pouw and his collaborator, Susanne Fuchs from Leibniz-Centre General Linguistics (ZAS) suggest that this is comparable to what happens when humans produce rhythmic hand gestures, as another line of research has shown that these hand gestures affect rib cage movements, which slightly change the volume of the lungs, and thereby change aspects of the voice.

    “Once we started reading up on very different lines of research, we were astonished how prevalent it is for animals to time their vocalizations together with movements. We discovered that coupling movement and vocalization might be very important for the development of vocal repertoires,” Dr. Fuchs says. In a particular kind of bird, for example, flightless chicklets have a very typical vocalization. But the vocalization quality drastically changes when the chicklets start using their wings in the nest to practice flying. Vocalization becomes similar to adult vocalizations when they start using their wings for flight. “This is exactly what is found in human infants around 9 months of age!” Pouw says, “when infants start to rhythmically “babble” with their hands in an exploratory fashion, they also start to “babble” in speech in the form of reduplicated syllables such as mama or papa”. Rhythmic movements may thus have very early connections with the voice, which explains why movement and sounds are so fundamentally related.

    This new perspective of how gesture relates to the voice via interactions with respiration provides an answer to why humans started to move their hands while vocalizing and speaking: These systems physically interacted throughout evolution, and this development is in a way mirrored by infants who start to learn to move and vocalize at the same time. “Many researchers of communication believe that manual gestures were for showing things,” Pouw says “but we actually think this not the whole story… in a way gestures are also part of what it means for humans to speak, which is not a passive affair, but an orchestra of movement to generate meaning”.

    The Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour at Radboud University is a world-class research centre devoted to understanding the mechanistic underpinnings of human cognition and behavior in health and disease. The Institute is home to more than 600 researchers from 35 countries who share the common goal of contributing to the advancement of the brain-, cognitive- and behavioral sciences through investigator-driven research, and improving health, education and technology by applying advances in this field.

    The Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft (ZAS) is a university-independent research institute located in the federal state of Berlin. Its objective is the investigation of natural language and its manifestation in individual languages. The aim is to better understand this central human ability and its biological, cognitive and social factors.


    Wissenschaftliche Ansprechpartner:

    Dr. Wim Pouw: wim.pouw@donders.ru.nl, Tel: 0031630030071
    Dr. Susanne Fuchs: fuchs@leibniz-zas.de, Tel: 004917646184175


    Originalpublikation:

    Pouw, W. & Fuchs, S. "Origins of vocal-entangled gesture." In: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104836


    Bilder

    Human gesture–speech biomechanics, and locomotor–respiratory–vocal coupling in a flying bat
    Human gesture–speech biomechanics, and locomotor–respiratory–vocal coupling in a flying bat

    Pouw, W. & Fuchs, S. "Origins of vocal-entangled gesture."


    Merkmale dieser Pressemitteilung:
    Journalisten, Wissenschaftler
    Biologie, Kulturwissenschaften, Medizin, Sprache / Literatur
    überregional
    Forschungsergebnisse, Wissenschaftliche Publikationen
    Englisch


     

    Human gesture–speech biomechanics, and locomotor–respiratory–vocal coupling in a flying bat


    Zum Download

    x

    Hilfe

    Die Suche / Erweiterte Suche im idw-Archiv
    Verknüpfungen

    Sie können Suchbegriffe mit und, oder und / oder nicht verknüpfen, z. B. Philo nicht logie.

    Klammern

    Verknüpfungen können Sie mit Klammern voneinander trennen, z. B. (Philo nicht logie) oder (Psycho und logie).

    Wortgruppen

    Zusammenhängende Worte werden als Wortgruppe gesucht, wenn Sie sie in Anführungsstriche setzen, z. B. „Bundesrepublik Deutschland“.

    Auswahlkriterien

    Die Erweiterte Suche können Sie auch nutzen, ohne Suchbegriffe einzugeben. Sie orientiert sich dann an den Kriterien, die Sie ausgewählt haben (z. B. nach dem Land oder dem Sachgebiet).

    Haben Sie in einer Kategorie kein Kriterium ausgewählt, wird die gesamte Kategorie durchsucht (z.B. alle Sachgebiete oder alle Länder).