idw – Informationsdienst Wissenschaft

Nachrichten, Termine, Experten

Grafik: idw-Logo
Science Video Project
idw-Abo

idw-News App:

AppStore

Google Play Store



Instanz:
Teilen: 
18.10.2022 10:50

The Mona Lisa effect: How eyespots deter predators that approach from different directions

Angela Overmeyer Presse- und Öffentlichkeitsarbeit
Max-Planck-Institut für chemische Ökologie

    Chicks are intimidated by eyespots that appear to gaze at them, providing an explanation for why eyespots are often symmetrical. In a new study, researchers from the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology and the University of Newcastle show that the configuration of eyespots have an impact on the prey’s survival.The most effective deterrent are concentric circles that subjectively appear to maintain eye contact regardless of the perspective from which they are approached

    The eyespots of many prey animals deter predators from attacking, like those of the peacock butterfly. In a new study, researchers from the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology and the University of Newcastle show that the configuration of eyespots have an impact on the prey’s survival. The deterrent potential of eyespots is larger when they appear to look directly at a predatory chick. The most effective deterrent are concentric circles that subjectively appear to maintain eye contact regardless of the perspective from which they are approached (Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, October 2022, doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.951967).

    Look me in the eyes: In nature a matter of life and death

    Visitors to an art gallery may be familiar with this effect: they feel watched or even followed by the portrayed person’s eyes everywhere in the room. This is called the "Mona Lisa effect" after the best-known portrait with the effect described, and it is created because the painter precisely centered the pupils of the portrayed person. Some animals, including many fish and butterflies, have paired circular spots on their bodies that closely resemble eyes. "Nature seems to utilize the Mona Lisa effect as well. But in the animal kingdom, it can be a matter of life and death," says Hannah Rowland, head of the Max Planck Research Group on Predators and Toxic Prey at the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology in Jena, Germany.

    The directions of the gaze matters

    To test the Mona Lisa effect and rule out a general deterrent effect of conspicuous patterns, Hannah Rowland and her colleague John Skelhorn from Newcastle University developed a behavioral experiment with newly hatched domestic chicks that they trained to attack artificial moths for a mealworm reward. When the chicks had learned how to attack the prey, they were presented with one of three different artificial moths: one with eyes whose middle circles looked to the left, one with “eyes” that looked to the right, and one with perfectly concentric circles; thus, they appeared to gaze straight forward or to one of the two sides. Then the researchers built mini catwalks (or rather chickwalks) that either led straight to the food or approached their prey from the side.

    The results of the behavioral experiments were unambiguous: "The chicks approached more cautiously from the left when the eyespots appeared to look to the left. Chicks approaching from the right showed similar caution when the eyespots were shifted to the right. However, when the chicks approached the artificial eyes from the opposite direction, they attacked the artificial moth quickly and ate the mealworm. Chicks approached moths with concentric circular eyes from all directions only with great caution," John Skelhorn summarizes the observations.

    Chicks perceive eyespots as eyes

    The behavioral observations confirm the prediction that the chicks perceive the artificial eyespots as eyes. "Eyespots with concentric circles seem to stare at potential predators, like our chicks, from many directions, just like portraits that seem to maintain eye contact no matter where you are in the room. This probably also explains why, in nature, eyespots have evolved independently in different animals to successfully deter enemies," believes Hannah Rowland.

    Hannah Rowland heads the independent Max Planck Research Group Predators and Toxic Prey at the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology in Jena. She focuses on predator-prey relationships, which have long served as models for the investigation of adaptation and fitness in natural environments, and provide some of the best examples of parallel evolution. Hannah Rowland and her team use tools from small molecule chemistry, computational biology, physiology, molecular and cell biology, and behavioral ecology to address these fundamental questions: How do new traits arise and become established in populations? How do traits vary across levels, i.e., among individuals within populations, and among populations? What are the factors that promote and maintain trait variability? Answers to these questions are useful for identifying the ecological and evolutionary processes that underpin interactions between organisms. They also help to understand the the origins of biodiversity and local adaptation, and how biological systems respond to challenges in their environment.


    Wissenschaftliche Ansprechpartner:

    Dr. Hannah Rowland, Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Max Planck Research Group on Predators and Toxic Prey, Hans-Knöll-Straße 8, 07745, Jena, Tel. +49 3641 57-1800, E-Mail hrowland@ice.mpg.de


    Originalpublikation:

    Skelhorn, J., Rowland, H. M. (2022). Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 10:951967. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.951967
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.951967


    Weitere Informationen:

    https://blog.frontiersin.org/2022/10/12/ever-watchful-eyes-how-moth-wing-pattern... Editorial by Hannah Rowlnd and John Skelhorn in Frontiers Science News
    https://www.ice.mpg.de/216803/predators-and-toxic-prey Max Planck Research Group Predators and Toxic Prey


    Bilder

    peacock butterfly
    peacock butterfly
    Hannah Rowland
    Hannah Rowland

    Predatory chicks and artificial peacock butterflies in a behavioral experiment
    Predatory chicks and artificial peacock butterflies in a behavioral experiment
    Hannah Rowland
    Hannah Rowland


    Merkmale dieser Pressemitteilung:
    Journalisten, Wissenschaftler
    Biologie, Tier / Land / Forst, Umwelt / Ökologie
    überregional
    Forschungsprojekte, Wissenschaftliche Publikationen
    Englisch


     

    peacock butterfly


    Zum Download

    x

    Predatory chicks and artificial peacock butterflies in a behavioral experiment


    Zum Download

    x

    Hilfe

    Die Suche / Erweiterte Suche im idw-Archiv
    Verknüpfungen

    Sie können Suchbegriffe mit und, oder und / oder nicht verknüpfen, z. B. Philo nicht logie.

    Klammern

    Verknüpfungen können Sie mit Klammern voneinander trennen, z. B. (Philo nicht logie) oder (Psycho und logie).

    Wortgruppen

    Zusammenhängende Worte werden als Wortgruppe gesucht, wenn Sie sie in Anführungsstriche setzen, z. B. „Bundesrepublik Deutschland“.

    Auswahlkriterien

    Die Erweiterte Suche können Sie auch nutzen, ohne Suchbegriffe einzugeben. Sie orientiert sich dann an den Kriterien, die Sie ausgewählt haben (z. B. nach dem Land oder dem Sachgebiet).

    Haben Sie in einer Kategorie kein Kriterium ausgewählt, wird die gesamte Kategorie durchsucht (z.B. alle Sachgebiete oder alle Länder).